Theoretical basis of combined movement theory

Chapter Two Theoretical basis of combined movement theory





Introduction


The physiological and psychological mechanisms of a treatment approach as diverse as ‘manual therapy’ are extraordinarily complex. An individual receiving manual therapy will be presenting for this treatment with a profile of characteristics (anxious, in pain, moving abnormally, experiencing some hypertonicity and associated muscle inhibition) that will be a representation of an infinite number of combinations of mechanisms. The influence of manual therapy on some of these mechanisms is understood whilst others have been insufficiently researched. The skill of the manual therapist is to interpret the profile of the individual patient and target manual therapy treatment towards the predominant mechanism of dysfunction. Some patients will have restricted movement due to joint stiffness or to muscle hypertonicity, others will be fearful of specific movements, whilst others may not be aware or concerned about how they excessively move a region of their spines.


This chapter details some of the underlying evidence to support the use of manual therapy and, in particular, the use of specific movement in the treatment of spinal pain. The mechanisms discussed below are by no means exclusive to, although they are particularly relevant to, the CMT approach. There is a certain mystique surrounding spinal manual therapy that does not follow manual treatment on the periphery. Attending for manual therapy to increase mobility and reduce pain following a fractured radius is not considered to be controversial, whilst attending for spinal manual therapy is often the source of considerable discussion.


Some of the reasons for this controversy are:



We are still developing a body of evidence to establish the physiological and psychological effects of manual therapy. Ultimately, manual therapy is a method of intervention for musculoskeletal dysfunction that encourages accelerated recovery. In light of this we must consider how we are measuring our effectiveness. The measurement of manual therapy against other treatments when judged in terms of change in disability, and reported 12 months after our intervention, may not be a sensitive method of capturing our effect (McCarthy & Cairns, 2005).




Spinal manipulation


Whilst there is considerable difference in individual practitioners’ approaches to manipulation there are some common features that define manipulation and mobilization. Some difference in practice may be professionally driven or by schools of thought within professions. It is worth considering that, even within the small world of manipulative therapy, we may mean very different things by the same terminology. A synopsis of the multi-disciplinary groups practising manual therapy leads us to a number of common features in a definition of manipulation:



Whilst there is huge debate about where in the range of movement a technique is performed the two features that are common to many definitions of manipulation are: (1) reference to motion at one functional spinal unit and (2) mention of an audible ‘popping’ sound.




Tearing adhesions in the capsule or stretching joints?


Imagine a scenario where your patient has attended with pain at three quarters range, right rotation, depicted in the top line of Figure 2.1. You feel a manipulative technique will be potentially of value in your management programme. The starting position you would adopt for the manipulative thrust would be formed by a combination of three physiological movements. In extension, ipsilateral flexion and contra-lateral flexion, the amount of rotation developed will be much less than the amount of rotation required to stretch or ‘tear’ an adhesion. Thus, it is unlikely that we are aiming to lengthen or tear tissue with our manipulative thrusts, whereas we feel we are aiming to lengthen tissues with our mobilization/stretching techniques.



Is the thrust element of the technique applied at end of range? Physiotherapists familiar with the Maitland Concept will be used to the nomenclature of grades of movement as described by Geoff Maitland (Maitland, 1986). Grades I and II refer to passive movement (of small and large amplitude respectively) performed in the early part of the range. In this range the therapist will not perceive resistance to movement. Grades III and IV are (large and small amplitude) movements conducted in a range where resistance is detected by the therapist. Small amplitude movement in early ranges of resistance are noted as IV− whilst movement at end of range as IV+. Manipulative thrusts techniques have been notated as Grade V, suggesting that the technique is conducted beyond the end of range and indeed some therapists may perform a high velocity thrust at the end of passive range. In common with many other manual therapists and osteopaths we feel there are disadvantages to performing manipulative thrusts at the end of passive range and numerous advantages to avoiding this starting position (Evans & Breen, 2006). This assertion is based on an understanding of the effects of passive manipulation and mobilization and represent an individual viewpoint on the objectives of the technique. However, there are many views on the objectives of manipulative technique and some manual therapists may feel they should conduct a manipulation as an end-of-range technique in order to stretch tissue.


One of the aims of manual therapy is to elongate soft tissue. In the process of stretching connective tissue a stress/strain curve has been identified that represents the behaviour of the tissue. Figure 2.2 depicts the relationship between the application of a strain on tissue and its resultant change in length. The relationship is speed specific with changes in the speed of application resulting in differing lengthening characteristics (Lederman, 2005).



Connective tissue that is loaded quickly will deform less than tissue loaded slowly, with the same amount of force. This is an important factor when considering manipulative thrusts and lengthening tissue. Figure 2.3 illustrates the fact that greater force is required to elongate tissue if applied quickly rather than slowly. Less force is required to lengthen hypomobile soft tissue if applied slowly, and as quick, forceful movements have greater potential to evoke nociceptive afferent barrage, the therapist should consider carefully the use of thrust techniques when deliberately trying to reduce hypomobility, particularly in the presence of pain.





What about the pop/cavitation?


In the 1970s, Sandoz conducted a series of experiments looking at distraction of finger joints. He noted that if cavitation occurred during distraction (signified by an audible popping sound), there was a gain of 5–10° in the range of passive movement of the metacarpophalangeal joint in all directions and the active range of movement was also increased, but to a lesser extent. Sandoz also noted that regardless of the direction of the distraction to the metacarpophalangeal joint, the refractory period remained the same and a radiolucent space appeared on the X-ray film. Sandoz (1976) went on to review what was known about the mechanical effects of joint cavitation at the time and produced a summary diagram (Fig. 2.4).



In Figure 2.4, two important features of the effects of distraction on a synovial joint can be identified. Firstly, the phenomenon of cavitation does not occur at the end of the joint’s distraction range but rather in the middle of range. Secondly, there is a change in the profile of resistance to movement exhibited in the range preceding cavitation and immediately following it. Post cavitation there is a reduction in the force required to distract the joint; it is less resistant to movement.


This leaves the question of whether cavitations at the finger joints are the same as those that occurred at the facet joint. Méal and Scott (1986) and Conway et al (1993) conducted studies examining the sound signals created by distractions of the fingers and those produced by spinal manipulation. Observing that the sound signals had similar acoustic nature led them to postulate that a similar mechanism was occurring and that joint gapping resulting in an increased range of movement was likely. Since then Cramer et al (2000) and Cramer et al (2002) have examined the effect of manipulation on the zygapophysial (facet) joints using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Following a successful pilot study on 16 healthy subjects they went on to randomize 64 healthy volunteers into four groups (Cramer et al, 2002). All participants had their initial scan whilst lying supine, then group one were placed in a side-lying posture and re-scanned. The second group had a manipulation and then were re-scanned in a supine position. The third group received a manipulation and were then placed in a side-lying position and re-scanned and finally, the fourth group were turned onto their side briefly and then re-scanned in a supine position. The second scan was conducted within 15–20 minutes of the intervention.


The authors demonstrated that the average change in joint separation following manipulation noted between the three radiologists blinded to the groups (intra and inter-rater reliability reported to be high with ICCs; 0.97 and 0.95 respectively) for the third group was +1.32 mm (manipulated then re-scanned in side-lying position), whereas for the second group (manipulated then re-scanned in a supine position), the average change was only +0.01 mm. In the final group the subjects were just placed in a side-lying posture and the change was −0.01 mm. They concluded that the results are consistent with the hypothesis that chiropractic manipulations gap the facet joint.


Sep 9, 2016 | Posted by in MANUAL THERAPIST | Comments Off on Theoretical basis of combined movement theory

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access