Chapter 14 Teaching and Learning Psychomotor Skills
Motor learning focuses on the processes to develop new strategies, to retain strategies, and to generalize strategies. An understanding of these motor learning principles is as important to the practitioner in physical therapy as are the elements of didactic and clinical teaching presented in Chapters 2, 3, and 8 to 11. The primary purpose of this chapter is to present variables related to motor learning that therapists can manipulate to facilitate acquisition of psychomotor skills.
After completing this chapter, the reader should be able to:
1. Differentiate between motor performance and motor learning.
2. Differentiate amongst Adams’, Schmidt’s, and Newell’s motor learning theories.
3. Describe processes that influence motor performance and motor learning.
4. Identify stages of motor learning as described by Fitts and Posner.
5. Discuss the application of Gentile’s taxonomy of tasks to physical therapy practice.
6. Describe individual, task, and environment influences on motor performance and motor learning.
7. Manipulate prepractice, practice, and feedback variables to optimize motor learning based on learner characteristics and task demands.
8. Adapt motor learning principles to meet the unique needs of various patient populations.
Distinction between motor learning and motor performance
Motor performance may be influenced by a number of variables, including motivation and the use of pharmacologic agents. Maturation and practice are factors that may influence both motor performance and motor learning. A common method of separating the permanent effects of maturation and practice is to measure changes across days or weeks instead of years. However, this method is often ineffective when attempting to measure learning in pediatric and elderly populations because maturation can result in significant changes over days or weeks in children and older adults. To separate maturation and practice influences on performance in these populations, comparisons of practice and nonpractice groups are often necessary. For example, several studies have used two group experimental designs to separate performance changes due to maturation and participation in early intervention programs.1,2
Overview of processes of motor learning
The processes of motor learning extend beyond conquering the mere motor demands of a task. Motor learning involves developing strategies to cope with the complexities of performing a specific task under specific environmental conditions. Learners must be able to solve motor problems (i.e., process information and engage in sensory encoding and memory retrieval processes) and respond to changes in the task and the environment. To optimize learning, therapists must provide patients with practice conditions that encourage (or possibly force) them to engage in problem-solving processes.3 This suggests that patients should be active participants not only in the production of their movements but also in planning their movements. Instead of providing patients with solutions to motor problems, therapists should act as educators and guide patients through learning processes that allow the patients to actively explore and discover solutions to motor problems.3 A summary of the processes of motor learning is given in Box 14–1.
Box 14–1 Summary of Processes of Motor Learning
• Learners remember processes, not specific movement patterns.
• Relative to guidance, problem solving enhances learning.
• The three sequential stages of learning are cognitive, associative, and autonomous.
• Automaticity develops by learning to focus on a critical subset of perceptual cues and motor strategies and by reorganizing information in units (termed chunking).
• The capability to detect and correct errors enhances learning. Error detection and correction occur online or during, slow, positioning movements. They occur after the movement in fast, timing tasks.
• Sensory and motor memories are thought to be stored in memory.
• Retrieval practice enhances learning more than repetitive drills.
• Instead of focusing on individual elements of a functional task, performers should focus on the goals of a task.
• With practice, actions become more efficient when performers learn to exploit the biomechanics of a task.
• Categorizing tasks based on task goals and environmental context and learner characteristics can enhance understanding of task requirements.
Motor learning theories
Various theories attempt to explain motor learning.4–6 Table 14–1 provides a brief summary of several popular theories: Adams’ Closed-Looped Theory,4 Schmidt’s Schema Theory,5 and Newell’s Ecological Theory.6 Each of these theories has limitations and varying levels of research support as well as clinical implications. For example, clinical application of Schmidt’s Schema Theory5 may consist of practicing a task under a variety of conditions to assist learners in developing a set of rules or schema related to the specific task. Application of Newell’s Ecological Theory6 would suggest that practicing a task under variable conditions will assist learners in understanding the relationship between perceptual cues and motor action (i.e., the glass looks heavy and therefore will require more force to lift).
Theory | Defining Characteristics |
---|---|
Adams’ Closed-Looped Theory | Sensory feedback is required for movement (now known to be false). Exemplar (or individual) sensory and motor memories are stored each time an action is performed. Enhancing sensory feedback will enhance learning. Errors will always interfere with learning (now known to be false). Emphasizes practicing tasks to be performed at a later time (termed specificity of learning). |
Schmidt’s Schema Theory | Defines a class of tasks as actions having identical relative timing and amplitude. Generalized sensory and motor memories are stored for a class of tasks. Novice actions should be performed as well as practiced actions within the same class of tasks. Errors can enhance learning. Emphasizes benefits of practicing several variations of a class of tasks (termed variability in practice). |
Newell’s Ecological Theory | Emphasizes performer, task, and environment constraints and relationships. Emphasizes relationships between sensory (perceptual) cues and motor (action) strategies. Emphasizes relationships between sensory and motor processes. Emphasizes the importance of variable practice related to task demands and environmental conditions. |
Stages of learning
According to Fitts and Posner,7 the process of motor learning can be divided into three sequential stages: (1) the cognitive stage, (2) the associative stage, and (3) the autonomous stage. During the cognitive stage, the learner focuses on understanding the task, developing strategies to execute the task, and determining ways to evaluate task success. Performance during this stage is often characterized by inaccuracies, slowness, and movements that appear stiff and uncoordinated. Because this stage is characterized by rapidly improving and variable performance, it is thought to require a high degree of attention and other cognitive processes.
Therapists can actively promote learning during the cognitive stage by facilitating the patient’s understanding of the task and organizing practice in ways that will specially encourage early learning.8 Emphasizing the purpose of the activity within a context that is functionally relevant to the individual patient may help the patient to better understand the task.8,9 Structuring the environment to reduce distractions may help the learner to better attend to learning. Clear, concise instructions should be provided in a manner that does not overwhelm the learner. Demonstrations of how the task should ideally be performed and providing hands-on guidance as needed will assist the patient in developing a cognitive map of the correct performance of the task. The use of feedback and practice schedules to promote learning at the various stages will be discussed later in this chapter.
After the initial cognitive stage, learners enter the associative stage of learning. Here, the goal is to fine-tune a skill. During this stage, the focus is on how to produce the most efficient action. Relative to the cognitive stage, this stage is characterized by slower gains in performance and reduced variability. To continue with the previous example of learning to drive a car, the first year or so of driving represents the associative stage of learning. After time, my daughter learned to smoothly accelerate and decelerate the car at intersections and to smoothly change gears using the gearshift, clutch, and gas pedals. The associative stage is represented in physical therapy when patients practice a skill to increase the safety or efficiency of a task. For example, when a person with a transfemoral amputation is learning to use a lower extremity prosthesis, the slow transition from taking a few uncoordinated steps to walking smoothly across the floor represents the associative stage. In essence, the patient needs practice time to enhance performance of the skill. Therapists can enhance learning in the associative stage by reducing the amount of hands-on guiding or assistance provided to the patient.8 The environment should be structured to gradually promote variations in practice conditions and demands.8
Error detection
The capability to detect errors is another process that is thought to develop with learning. Error detection capabilities are thought to require memory of sensory feedback from previously performed actions and are used differently for slow-positioning and fast-timing tasks. In slow-positioning tasks, sensory feedback is used to guide the action to its endpoint. Thus, learners move until feedback from the present action matches the memory of sensory feedback for the desired action. In fast-timing tasks, learners are unable to use sensory feedback to alter an action online, or during an action. In such tasks, sensory feedback is used to detect errors after the action has ended.10
Motor memories
Memory is an essential aspect of effective motor learning.11 Three distinct memory systems are thought to exist: short-term sensory store (STSS), short-term memory (STM), and long-term memory (LTM).11 In STSS, numerous segments or streams of sensory information entering the system are briefly stored by sensory modality (e.g., visual, tactile, kinesthetic). Information in STSS is not thought to reach a conscious level and is only retained for a matter of milliseconds. Selected information from STSS is selected for further processing in STM. Selection is thought to be based on the relevance and pertinence of the information. Some authors conceptualize STM as a temporary workspace and may term STM as a form of working memory.11 People hold onto information in STM for only as long as they direct their attention to the information.
LTM is memory system that stores information and experiences accumulated over a life time.11 LTM is thought to consist of two basic forms: declarative (explicit) learning and nondeclarative (implicit) learning. Explicit learning is associated with knowledge that can be consciously recalled and stated, such as facts and events. It requires attention, awareness, and reflection.12 Implicit learning refers to memories that are less accessible to conscious recollection and verbal recall. Several forms of implicit learning are exemplified by fairly passive learning processes in which learners acquire knowledge through exposure to information. The attentional and cognitive demands of such learning processes are limited. Implicit learning includes the following forms of learning: nonassociative, associative, and procedural.11,12
Nonassociative learning occurs when individuals are repeatedly exposed to a single stimulus.11 Simple forms of nonassociative learning include habituation and sensitization and are often utilized in clinical practice. For example, patients with certain vestibular disorders have been shown to benefit from exercises that center on repeated performance of activities that provoke their dizziness.13,14 Although the neuromechanisms behind habituation are not fully understood, over time, these patients habituate to the stimulus and experience a reduction in their symptoms.13,14
Through associative learning, a person learns to predict relationships such as the relationship of one stimulus to another (classical conditioning) or the relationship of a behavior to a consequence (operant conditioning).11 In therapy sessions, if we repeatedly provide a patient with a verbal cue and a physical prompt when performing a transfer, we may see that over time, the patient will begin to associate the verbal and physical cueing and begin to perform the transfer with verbal cues only. In operant conditioning, a patient may learn that behaviors leading to rewards should be repeated and that behaviors that have negative results should be avoided. This may be a factor for our elderly patients who have recently experienced a fall (a negative consequence) in that such patients may choose to decrease their activity in order to decrease their chances of falling again. Fear of falling may thus become an obstacle that must be conquered in our therapy sessions.
Procedural learning relates to learning tasks though intense practice to the point at which the task can be performed without conscious thought or active attention.11 Such learning occurs with repetition across varying environmental conditions and happens gradually over a period of time. Once the learner learns the “rules” to performing the task, the task is executed with very little conscious attention.11 In therapy, achieving such comfort with motor skills is often a desired outcome. For example, when teaching a patient with a spinal cord injury to perform transfers, our long-term goal may relate to achieving a level of automaticity and generalizability that will allow the patient to transfer safely under a variety of plausible conditions.
Many intervention approaches rely on explicit learning techniques. When teaching crutch use on stairs, for example, therapists often use strategies that involve explicit directions: “Up with the right foot; down with the left foot.” When gait-training with a patient, we often give the patient verbal instructions: “Take a longer step on the right” or “Lift your knee higher.” For patients who are able to process verbal information, maintain attention, and concurrently perform motor and cognitive tasks (listening to the therapist while walking), such strategies may be successful and appropriate. Yet for patients who have cognitive, attentional, or language deficits, such explicit learning strategies may pose difficulties. For example, Orrell and coworkers15 compared explicit and implicit learning strategies for teaching a dynamic balance task and found that the provision of explicit information may actually be detrimental for some patients after a stroke. A study by Boyd and Winstein16 further found that regardless of whether subjects had sustained a stoke involving the basal ganglia or the sensorimotor cortex, the provision of explicit information had a negative impact on learning and skill retention.
Focusing on actions, not movements
Many motor behaviorists argue that memories for movements focus on task goals.17 There is little evidence that learners store and retrieve memories for individual segments of an action (e.g., extend the elbow, open the fingers, close the fingers, then grasp an object), without regard for the task goal or the environment. This principle suggests that patients should practice tasks or actions, not individual movements. For example, a child with cerebral palsy may be learning to ride a tricycle during therapy sessions. The therapeutic goal may be to enhance interlimb coordination between her legs. During practice, however, the therapist and child focus on an outcome goal (moving the tricycle forward as fast as possible), and not on the movements required for interlimb coordination.
Learning to exploit biomechanics
Increased consistency in kinematics and coordination also occurs with practice. Learners are taught to discover ways to take advantage of the passive inertia properties of muscles, joints, and limbs.3 With practice, performers demonstrate increased speed and decreased energy costs because they have learned to optimize the peripheral sensory and motor requirements of the task. Therapists must be able to help patients exploit biomechanics to achieve a goal. This is especially important during the associative stage of learning when patients are trying to fine-tune a skill and should be exposed to different variations of the same skill. For example, therapists most often teach a force-control strategy for sit-to-stand transfers. Although this strategy is relatively safe, a momentum strategy is more efficient.9 Shumway-Cook and Woollacott9 advocate that patients be allowed to explore several strategies for transfers so that they have choices available. When patients seek safety over efficiency, they may choose a force-control strategy, whereas when efficiency is the primary goal, a momentum strategy may be chosen.