The Functional Information-processing Model

9. The Functional Information-processing Model

Jackie Pool


Overview
This chapter describes the work of Claudia Kay Hoover Allen, an American occupational therapist, who developed the Cognitive Disability Model, which was later refined and renamed as the Functional Information-processing Model (FIPM). This is an occupational therapy model that is used when working with people with cognitive impairments, including those caused by dementia, developmental delay, mental health problems, such as depression or schizophrenia, and acquired brain injury. The FIPM proposes that there are six cognitive levels, each of which can be described in terms of limitations associated with the medical condition and remaining abilities, as seen in patterns of behaviour in everyday tasks. The levels range from level 6, where functioning is normal and no intervention is required, to level 1, where the person is profoundly impaired. An analysis of the results aids understanding of the patient’s behaviour when carrying out everyday tasks. Optimum environmental stimulation and support can then be planned and implemented to decrease confusion, maximize functional capacities and help the person retain a sense of competence despite impairment.

The theoretical basis of the FIPM and its relevance to occupational therapists are explored and the levels of cognitive ability within the model are defined. A battery of assessments based on the model are outlined and information about their reliability and validity is provided. The chapter explores the application of the FIPM to practice.






Introduction



In the USA the FIPM has been adopted for use to meet the requirements of Medicare. In 1965, the Social Security Act established Medicare as a prepaid medical insurance plan extending health coverage to almost all Americans aged 65 or older. Medicare requires that rehabilitation goals be functional, practical, sustainable and completed in a reasonable period of time. For the people with longer-term needs, goals must establish safe and effective maintenance programmes that will be sustained after therapy is discontinued. Allen proposed that the battery of assessments help occupational therapists to meet the Medicare requirements with rehabilitation goals that are relevant, reasonable and possible.

Historically, the cognitive and physical components of people have been assessed and treated separately. While there are many assessments for determining cognitive disability, the Allen’s Cognitive Levels constitutes one that defines cognitive functioning in the way that it affects the person’s engagement in occupation and with others. It is useful to all disciplines in the rehabilitation department, and in both the USA and the UK is used by occupational therapists to contribute to the findings of multidisciplinary teams. Although the FIPM and the battery of assessments have been developed for use by occupational therapists, they are also viewed by Allen as appropriate for use by other therapists, such as physiotherapists, who have an understanding of neuropsychology and experience in working with people having impairments of information-processing.

In May 2001, the World Health Organization (WHO 2002) endorsed the use of the International Classification of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF). The ICF is a multi-purpose classification of health and health-related domains that helps to describe changes in body function and structure, and what a person with a health condition can do in a standard environment (their level of capacity), as well as what they actually do in their usual environment (their level of performance). These domains are classified from body, individual and societal perspectives by means of two components: body functions and structure, and activity and participation. The ICF also lists environmental factors that interact with all of these components (WHO 2002). Allen’s FIPM fits well with the ICF, as the assessments aim to identify both level of cognitive capacity and level of performance. Interpretation of the assessment findings guides the occupational therapist to create a helping, therapeutic environment that meets the individual’s needs and at the same time enables the individual to use their existing abilities to engage with their world.

The Allen assessment battery is now used widely in the UK by occupational therapists in a variety of clinical settings. The main users are those working with older people with dementia, because this has been the field where information about the FIPM has been most widely disseminated. The older persons’ specialist section of the UK College of Occupational Therapists (COTSS-OP) has created a dementia clinical forum where information and workshops about Allen’s model and materials have been featured. Increasingly, occupational therapists from other fields, including forensic psychiatry, adult psychiatry and learning disability, are also beginning to use the FIPM in practice.

In the USA, there are two websites dedicated to Allen’s modules. Allen’s Conferences, Inc. (http://www.allen-cognitive-levels.com/) is the official website of Allen’s Cognitive Levels and information about their products and some research is available. More recently a website, the Allen Cognitive Network (http://www.allen-cognitive-network.org/), has been developed by an association of Allen Cognitive Level advisors, whose aim is to network about the Allen Cognitive Battery and applications in various practice arenas.


Theory


Cognition may be defined as the full range of processes and mechanisms that support thinking, as well as the thoughts themselves, which can be viewed as the products of those processes. This definition encompasses cognitive skills, such as attention, concentration, memory, comprehension, reasoning and problem-solving, as well as the cognitive processes, including styles of thinking, interpretations, judgements and assumptions (O’Neill 2002). Allen proposes that cognition is the processing capacity that defines what a person pays attention to, their motor response and their verbal performance. This processing capacity determines how a person engages in everyday activities and with their environment.

The clinical reasoning of the FIPM is based on the scientific assumption that human functioning is a general qualitative capacity to use mental energy to guide motor and verbal performance. Cognitive and physical components are intrinsically related since the brain guides physical behaviour. Allen suggests that cognitive disability is viewed as ‘a restriction in voluntary motor action originating in the physical or chemical structures of the brain and producing observable limitations in routine task behaviour’ (Allen 1985, p 31). These motor actions are the movements that a person makes as they are carrying out their everyday activities. Allen is therefore proposing that an individual’s cognition can be assessed by observing their engagement in everyday activities.

Allen’s early work was influenced by Piaget’s theory of cognitive development and she attributes the idea that there are six possible levels of cognition to Piaget’s sensorimotor period (Allen 1985). She later developed her theory to include the influence of the Soviet psychologist Vygotsky, whose work also focused on activity (Vygotsky 1978).

Allen describes a patient’s cognitive progress as a continuum along two paths, motor performance and verbal performance, which are linked by attention. Attention is defined as the way that information is noticed and used. Use of information can be expressed in motor and verbal performance, in isolation or simultaneously. In order to understand a person’s ability to function, Allen proposes that the therapist must first have an understanding of cognitive processing. Because the variation in human behaviour is so diverse, and there are so many variables resulting from unique experiences and personality, Allen bases FIPM on the structuralism of Piaget. Structures are the mental components used to organize thinking and learning processes; the selection of these components is influenced by the purpose for thinking and learning. Allen has defined the structures in broad, general terms that are intended to be universal and therefore free from gender and cultural bias. These structures, or processes, guide the qualitative differences in the actions and activities of individuals, and can be organized as observations, speed, visual spatial, verbal prepositional and memory.




Observations include attention to cues or external stimuli and making sense of cues. The understanding of actions and activities as producers of primary and secondary effects may also be viewed as observations.


Speed refers to the rate at which the information-processing system operates.


Visual spatial processes of working memory that are applied to understanding objects and space include sensation, perception, topographical orientation and imagination.


Verbal propositional processes of working memory that are applied to understanding communication, social order and time include non-verbal communication, verbal communication, cause and effect relationships where relationships are transferred into sounds, classifications of objects by perceptual properties, functional use or abstract concepts, and orientation to time and social rules.


Memory processes are divided into declarative (explicit) intentions to learn, and non-declarative (implicit) learning that occurs without being aware of storing or retrieving knowledge.

The conceptual framework of Allen’s FIPM is based on an understanding of these cognitive structures and the remaining abilities of the information-processing system. The model is designed to clarify remaining capacities in a disabled brain and is therefore an abilities-focused model.


Allen’s Cognitive Level Scale


Claudia Allen proposed six cognitive levels ranging from ‘coma’ (0.8) to ‘normal’ (6.0) (Allen et al., 1992 and Katz, 1998). Each level has three components: attention, motor control, and verbal performance. Table 9.1 summarizes the components of cognitive performance and the resulting impact on functional ability at each of these levels.



















































































Table 9.1 Cognitive performance and impact on functional ability

Level 1 Level 2 Level 3 Level 4 Level 5 Level 6
Sensory cues Subliminal Proprioceptive Tactile Visible Related Symbolic
Information-processing Reflex Effect on body Effect on environment Several actions Overt trial and error Covert trial and error
Motor actions Automatic Postural Manual Goal-directed Exploratory Planned
Reason for response Arousal Comfort/movement Interest
Touching
Compliance
Seeing
Self-control
Reasoning
Reflection
Reasoning
Perception of objects Penetrate subliminal state Own body Exterior surfaces Colour
Shape
Space and depth Intangible
Setting of objects Reflex zones Range of motion Arm’s reach Visual field Task environment Potential task environment
Use of objects Stimulated body part Spontaneous use of body part Chance use of found objects Hand tools as a means to an end Hand tools used to vary means and ends Tool-making
Verbal directions Verbs Pronouns
Names of body parts
Names of material objects Adjectives
Adverbs
Prepositions
Explanations
Conjunction
Conjectures
Demonstrated directions Physical contact Guided movements Action on an object Each step in a series Each step and precautions for errors Not required

Allen (1996) summarizes the functional implications of the cognitive levels as:




Level 0. The individual is alive but in a coma or under general anaesthesia. No conscious control of movement is evident.


Level 1. The individual responds to an external stimulus. A general response, like a change in heart rate, usually precedes a specific response to noxious stimuli, followed by additional stimuli like bells, voices, pictures and mobiles.


Level 2. The individual controls gross body movements to sit up, stand up, walk and do push/pull exercises. Adaptive equipment that protects the individual from hazardous postural movements or supports functional position is indicated.


Level 3. The hands are used to reach for and grasp objects. Repetitive manual actions are common, but the effect produced on the object is not judged. Constant supervision is required to protect the patient from harm. Activity that requires repetitive actions may sustain attention. Surprise at having produced an end result may occur.


Level 4. Actions are goal-directed to complete a familiar activity. The routine activities of daily living can be done independently. Assistance is required to solve any problems presented by changes in the environment and to protect from unseen hazards. Simple projects, with rules to follow, are preferred. Striking visual cues, like primary colours and familiar shapes, are matched according to a sample project.


Level 5. New actions are learned by doing an activity. The novelty presented by new products is explored. Hazards are not anticipated, and supervision in using dangerous or expensive products is advised. Aesthetic judgements about less striking visual cues are made, but with difficulty.


Level 6. The individual anticipates the consequences of his or her actions. An effective and efficient course of action is planned. The creation of an individual design can be premeditated and accomplished with ease. The fun of discovery and creativity is blended with discipline of good craftsmanship.

The difference between levels 1 and 2 is the ability to attend to and grasp moving objects. Between levels 2 and 3 there is a difference in the ability to manipulate objects in a meaningful way.

In order to discriminate further between the levels, five modes of performance have been added to each cognitive level to allow the therapist to locate the patient’s function level more precisely. The five modes are each in a scale that increases by 2 points, i.e. 1.0, 1.2, 1.4, 1.6 and 1.8. At mode .0 the patient is likely to be functioning in only some of the described manner; at mode .2 the person is characteristically more likely to have problems of orientation in time and place. Mode .4 is a consolidation and the classic description of the level, whereas at mode .6 the person is likely to be open to the next level as thought orientation shifts up. At mode .8 the person is working at a composite level by adding information from the next level.

Allen states that an interval scale, which assumes equal distances between all points on the scale, is inappropriate when dealing with all the variables of human behaviour. Some abilities to function have a greater meaning to some individuals than others, and this will affect individual performance as much as will the integrity of the cognitive structures and the ability to process information. Allen’s Cognitive Level Scale is therefore ordinal. This means that there is not an equal distance between each mode, there is a larger distance in ability between point 4 and point 6 of a mode, and between point 8 and point 0 of a mode, than between any of the others.

Only gold members can continue reading. Log In or Register to continue

Stay updated, free articles. Join our Telegram channel

Sep 9, 2016 | Posted by in MANUAL THERAPIST | Comments Off on The Functional Information-processing Model

Full access? Get Clinical Tree

Get Clinical Tree app for offline access